Learner and native language data

Native speaker and learner language data in Applied Linguistics: what’s next?

Monday 08 February 2016, 17:00-18:30
Faculty of Education, 184 Hills Road, Cambridge, CB2 8PQ, Room GS4.

Abstract

The use of corpus data is becoming increasingly popular in applied linguistics research and, particularly, with Second Language Acquisition (SLA) researchers. It is precisely now 10 years that Florence Myles published a seminal paper that called for the need to develop corpus resources that could meet the research needs of those in the field of SLA. Since then, a more fruitful dialogue has emerged between SLA researchers and corpus linguists.

In essence, corpora are principled collections of texts gathered with some clear purpose in mind, typically language research. A corpus is always motivated and its design principles are robust, which excludes the idea that any set of texts may qualify as a corpus.

In this talk I will deal with some guiding principles in corpus design and use and, in particular, will discuss the uses of both native and learner corpora in different research scenarios. In doing so, I would like to envisage future uses and developments at the crossroads of corpus and applied linguistics.

:::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::

 

Historian Larry Siedentop has written in his book Inventing the Individual the following: “The way humans understand themselves never captures the whole truth. It selects, simplifies and at times it distorts”. Is it then possible to transcend this apparently inherent human quality? Can we possibly move away from selection, oversimplification and distortion? Could we ever dump our “linguistic” armchairs? Do we need to?

Tony McEnery has outlined the reasons why corpus linguistics was largely ignored in the past possibly because of the influence of Noam Chomsky. Prof. McEnery has placed this debate in a wider context where different stakeholders fight a paradigm war: rationalist introspection versus evidence driven analysis.

“Science is a subject that relies on measurement rather than opinion”, Bill Cox wrote in the book version of Human Universe, the BBC Show. And I think he is right. Complementary research methodologies can only bring about better insights and better-informed debates.

:::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::::

Learner corpora around the world:

http://www.uclouvain.be/en-cecl-lcworld.html

:::::::::::::::::::::::::

Quote 1

The word corpus is Latin for body (plural corpora). In linguistics a corpus is a collection of texts (a ‘body’ of language) stored in an electronic database. Corpora are usually large bodies of machine-readable text containing thousands or millions of words. A corpus is different from an archive in that often (but not always) the texts have been selected so that they can be said to be representative of a particular language variety or genre, therefore acting as a standard reference. Corpora are often annotated with additional information such as part-of-speech tags or to denote prosodic features associated with speech. Individual texts within a corpus usually receive some form of meta-encoding in a header, giving information about their genre, the author, date and place of publication etc. Types of corpora include specialised, reference, multilingual, parallel, learner, diachronic and monitor. Corpora can be used for both quantitative and qualitative analyses. Although a corpus does not contain new information about language, by using software packages which process data we can obtain a new perspective on the familiar (Hunston 2002: 2–3).

Baker et al. (2006). A glossary of corpus linguistics. Edinburgh: UEP.

 

Quote 2

Armchair linguistics does not have a good name in some linguistics circles. A caricature of the armchair linguist is something like this. He sits in a deep soft comfortable armchair, with his eyes closed and his hands clasped behind his head. Once in a while he opens his eyes, sits up abruptly shouting, “Wow, what a neat fact!”, grabs his pencil, and writes something down. Then he paces around for a few hours in the excitement of having come still closer to knowing what language is really like. (There isn’t anybody exactly like this, but there are some approximations.)

Charles Fillmore. Directions in Corpus Linguistics (Proceedings of Nobel Symposium 82, 1991),

 

Quote 3

We as linguists should train ourselves specifically to be open to the evidence of long text. This is quite different from using the computer to be our servant in trying out our ideas; it is making good use of some essential differences between computers and people.

[…] I believe that we have to cultivate a new relationship between the ideas we have and the evidence that is in front of us. We are so used to interpreting very scant evidence that we are not in a good mental state to appreciate the opposite situation. With the new evidence the main difficulty is controlling and organizing it rather than getting it.

Sinclair. Trust the Text. (2004:17)

 

Quote 4

Register, genre, and style differences are fundamentally important for any student with a primary interest in language. For example, any student majoring in English, or in the study of another language like Japanese or Spanish, must understand the text varieties in that language. If you are training to become a teacher (e.g. for secondary education or for TESL), you will shortly be faced with the task of teaching your own students how to use the words and structures that are appropriate to different spoken and written tasks – different registers and genres. Other students of language are more interested in the study of literature or the creative writing of new literature, issues relating to the style perspective, since the literary effects that distinguish one novel (or poem) from the next are realized as linguistic differences.

Biber & Conrad  (2009:4)

 

Quote 5

However, even though language in Applied Linguistics is seen as a linguistic, social, cultural, political, an aesthetic, and an educational local practice, which the researcher is called upon to illuminate, in my experience, the practice itself is often used by young researchers to uncritically illustrate a theory born elsewhere.
In order to be seen as legitimate scholars, young applied linguists are encouraged to read theory, preferably of the French kind, and ‘apply’ it to their data (see McNamara 2015). But rather than being inspired by the practice to ask new and critical questions about the theory, that would ultimately benefit both the practice and the theory, they too often give their data short shrift by merely translating the practice into the nomenclature of an existing theory (see Shuy 2015). This might increase the symbolic value of applied linguistic research, but not necessarily its value for the practice.

Kramsch (2015:456-7)

 

Myles (2005)  on the future of learner corpora

Not only do we need large datasets in order to be able to generalize our findings, but some of the structures which are crucial for informing current debates are rarely found in learner data.

My concern is that the kind of studies that are being undertaken are too closely dependent on what corpora are at hand, and what software tools are available. (p.388)
The field needs to become much more ambitious in its use of new technologies, and in the kind of cor- pora it collects in order to address its current research agenda. (p. 388)

For this purpose, we need good quality longitudinal oral corpora, in a range of different L1/L2 combinations, for the reasons outlined in Section II. The possibilities offered by the computerized analysis of corpora are considerable, as I hope to have demonstrated. SLA researchers, however, need to make sure that not only the corpora they collect but also the computerized tools they use are adapted to their research agendas, rather than the other way round, i.e., adapting their research questions to the corpora or the tools readily available. Some sophisti- cated tools can be used, and it is high time that the pioneering work of L1 acquisitionists in this area is emulated by L2 researchers. (p.388)

Myles, F. (2005). Interlanguage corpora and second language acquisition research. Second Language Research, 21(4), 373-391.

 

Quote 6

Several reasons can be given for why elicitation techniques are favoured in SLA research. For instance, Mackey & Gass (2005) provide the following reasons why metalinguistic data may be used in SLA research, as opposed to natural lan- guage use data: (i) the particular structure you want to investigate may not occur in natural production: it may be absent or there may not be enough instances, and, conversely, (ii) to answer your research question you may need to know what learners rule out as a possible L2 sentence: (a) presence of a particular structure/ feature in the learners’ natural output does not necessarily indicate that the learn- ers know (i.e. have a mental representation of) the structure, and (b) absence of a particular structure/feature in natural language use data does not necessarily indi- cate that learners do not know the structure. An additional reason is provided by Granger (2002: 6): it is di cult to control the variables that a ect learner produc- tion in a non-experimental context. Additionally, L2 researchers have been typi- cally trained in (quasi)experimental methods rather than in corpus methods, ex- cept for those studies conducted with source data from CHILDES (see Myles 2007b: 386 for a discussion).  e consequence of all this is that the empirical base of SLA research tends to be relatively narrow, based on the language produced by a very limited number of subjects, which, as pointed out by Granger (2002: 6), raises questions about whether results can be generalised. But the methodological future of SLA looks promising, since some researchers are currently claiming that combining both naturalistic and experimental data is crucial to gain insight into the relation between the two types of data (e.g. Gilquin & Gries 2009).

Lozano, C., & Mendikoetxea, A. (2013). Learner corpora and Second Language Acquisition: The design and collection of CEDEL2. In A. Díaz-Negrillo, N. Ballier & P. Thompson (Eds.), Automatic Treatment and Analysis of Learner Corpus Data. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 65-100.

Quote 7

All in all, the contribution of learner corpus research so far has been much more substantial in description than interpretation of SLA data, documenting dif- ferences between native and non-native English, rather than explaining and ad- dressing the key theoretical issues in SLA research (Granger 2004; Myles 2005). According to Granger (2004: 134–135), this is because learner corpus research has been mainly conducted by corpus linguists, rather than SLA specialists (Hasselgard 1999), and the type of learner language corpus that researchers have been most interested in (intermediate to advanced) was so poorly described in the literature that they felt the need to establish the facts before launching into theoretical gen- eralisations.

As Tono summarises:
Many corpus-based researchers do not know enough about the theoretical back- ground of SLA research to communicate with them [i.e. SLA researchers] e ec- tively, while SLA researchers typically know little about what corpora can do for them. (Tono 2003: 806)

 

Lozano, C., & Mendikoetxea, A. (2013). Learner corpora and Second Language Acquisition: The design and collection of CEDEL2. In A. Díaz-Negrillo, N. Ballier & P. Thompson (Eds.), Automatic Treatment and Analysis of Learner Corpus Data. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 65-100.